Can you solve the false positive riddle? - Alex Gendler

پکیج: آموزشگاه تد / سرفصل: چیستان / درس 22

آموزشگاه تد

13 سرفصل | 232 درس

Can you solve the false positive riddle? - Alex Gendler

توضیح مختصر

Practice more problem-solving at https-//brilliant.org/TedEd/ Solution to the bonus riddle mentioned at the end- https-//brilliant.org/tededprobabilitypairs/ Mining unobtainium is hard work - the rare mineral appears in only 1% of rocks in the mine. But your friend Tricky Joe has something up his sleeve. The unobtainium detector he's been perfecting for months is finally ready, and it returns accurate readings 90% of the time. But can it really be trusted? Alex Gendler explains the false positive paradox. Lesson by Alex Gendler, animation by Artrake Studio. Thank you so much to our patrons for supporting us on Patreon! Without you this video would not be possible! Tan YH, Brittiny Elman, Mayra Urbano, Ruth Fang, Kostadin Mandulov, Alex Schenkman, Sdiep Sriram, Ivan Todorovic, Antero Semi, Yanuar Ashari, Mrinalini, Anthony Kudolo, Scott Gass, Querida Owens, Hazel Lam, Manav parmar, Dwight Tevuk, Stephen A. Wilson, Siamak H, Dominik Kugelmann, Katie Winchester, Mary Sawyer, David Rosario, Samuel Doerle, Be Owusu, Susan Herder, Savannah Scheelings, Prasanth Mathialagan, Yanira Santamaria, Chad Harper, Dawn Jordan, Constantin Salagor, Activated Classroom Teaching, Kevin Wong, Umar Farooq, Goh Xiang Ting Diana, Dmitry Neverov, Tushar Sharma, Cristobal Medina Moenne, MJ Tan Mingjie, Yansong Li, Jason A Saslow, Joanne Luce, Henry Li, Kyle Nguyen, Taylor Hunter, Noa Shore, Lex Azevedo, Merit Gamertsfelder, and Bev Millar.

  • زمان مطالعه 0 دقیقه
  • سطح خیلی سخت

دانلود اپلیکیشن «زوم»

این درس را می‌توانید به بهترین شکل و با امکانات عالی در اپلیکیشن «زوم» بخوانید

دانلود اپلیکیشن «زوم»

فایل ویدیویی

برای دسترسی به این محتوا بایستی اپلیکیشن زبانشناس را نصب کنید.

متن انگلیسی درس

Mining unobtainium is hard work.

The rare mineral appears in only 1% of rocks in the mine.

But your friend Tricky Joe has something up his sleeve.

The unobtainium detector he’s been perfecting for months is finally ready.

The device never fails to detect unobtainium if any is present.

Otherwise, it’s still highly reliable, returning accurate readings 90% of the time.

On his first day trying it out in the field, the device goes off, and Joe happily places the rock in his cart.

As the two of you head back to camp where the ore can be examined, Joe makes you an offer: he’ll sell you the ore for just $200.

You know that a piece of unobtanium that size would easily be worth $1000, but any other minerals would be effectively worthless.

Should you make the trade?

Pause here if you want to figure it out for yourself.

Answer in: 3 Answer in: 2 Answer in: 1 Intuitively, it seems like a good deal.

Since the detector is correct most of the time, shouldn’t you be able to trust its reading?

Unfortunately, no.

Here’s why.

Imagine the mine has exactly 1,000 pieces of ore.

An unobtainium rarity of 1% means that there are only 10 rocks with the precious mineral inside.

All 10 would set off the detector.

But what about the other 990 rocks without unobtainium?

Well, 90% of them, 891 rocks, to be exact, won’t set off anything.

But 10%, or 99 rocks, will set off the detector despite not having unobtanium, a result known as a false positive.

Why does that matter?

Because it means that all in all, 109 rocks will have triggered the detector.

And Joe’s rock could be any one of them, from the 10 that contain the mineral to the 99 that don’t, which means the chances of it containing unobtainium are 10 out of 109 – about 9%.

And paying $200 for a 9% chance of getting $1000 isn’t great odds.

So why is this result so unexpected, and why did Joe’s rock seem like such a sure bet?

The key is something called the base rate fallacy.

While we’re focused on the relatively high accuracy of the detector, our intuition makes us forget to account for how rare the unobtanium was in the first place.

But because the device’s error rate of 10% is still higher than the mineral’s overall occurrence, any time it goes off is still more likely to be a false positive than a real finding.

This problem is an example of conditional probability.

The answer lies neither in the overall chance of finding unobtainium, nor the overall chance of receiving a false positive reading.

This kind of background information that we’re given before anything happens is known as unconditional, or prior probability.

What we’re looking for, though, is the chance of finding unobtainium once we know that the device did return a positive reading.

This is known as the conditional, or posterior probability, determined once the possibilities have been narrowed down through observation.

Many people are confused by the false positive paradox because we have a bias for focusing on specific information over the more general, especially when immediate decisions come into play.

And while in many cases it’s better to be safe than sorry, false positives can have real negative consequences.

False positives in medical testing are preferable to false negatives, but they can still lead to stress or unnecessary treatment.

And false positives in mass surveillance can cause innocent people to be wrongfully arrested, jailed, or worse.

As for this case, the one thing you can be positive about is that Tricky Joe is trying to take you for a ride.

مشارکت کنندگان در این صفحه

تا کنون فردی در بازسازی این صفحه مشارکت نداشته است.

🖊 شما نیز می‌توانید برای مشارکت در ترجمه‌ی این صفحه یا اصلاح متن انگلیسی، به این لینک مراجعه بفرمایید.